Mostrar el registro sencillo del ítem
Intentional Variation in Morally Relevant Theory of Mind (MoToM) Tasks
Variación intencional en tareas de Teoría de la Mente Moralmente relevante (MoToM)
dc.creator | Goenaga, Jesús | |
dc.creator | Sierra, Daniel | |
dc.creator | Gallego, Federico | |
dc.creator | Artunduaga, Karen | |
dc.date | 2020-06-05 | |
dc.date.accessioned | 2020-08-21T19:43:37Z | |
dc.date.available | 2020-08-21T19:43:37Z | |
dc.identifier | https://revistas.unbosque.edu.co/index.php/CHP/article/view/3050 | |
dc.identifier | 10.18270/chps..v18i2.3050 | |
dc.identifier.uri | http://test.repositoriodigital.com:8080/handle/123456789/9855 | |
dc.description | The relationship between the Theory of Mind (ToM) and moral judgment allowed the design of Morally Relevant Theory of Mind (MoToM) tasks, which integrate intentional reading to the elaboration of such judgments. This paper describes the production of a MoToM variation in the moral judgment with intentional reading by children of 5 and 8 years old. They applied a MoToM task and its variation, giving two estimates of moral judgment both before and after the variation. Significant differences were found between ages before the elaboration of the variation. The relationship between ToM and moral judgment was confirmed, recognizing other aspects of development which are significant for the integration of intentionality in making judgments. | en-US |
dc.description | La relación entre Teoría de la Mente (ToM) y juicio moral permitió el diseño de tareas de Teoría de la Mente Moralmente Relevantes (MoToM), las cuales integran la lectura intencional a la elaboración de dichos juicios. Este trabajo describe la producción de una variación de la MoToM en el juicio moral con lectura intencional en niños de 5 y 8 años, quienes aplicaron una tarea de MoToM y variación de esta, otorgando dos estimaciones del juicio moral, antes y después de la variación. Se encontraron diferencias significativas entre edades ante la elaboración del juicio. Se confirmó la relación entre ToM y juicio moral, reconociendo otros aspectos del desarrollo, los cuales son fundamentales para la integración de la intencionalidad en la elaboración de juicios. | es-ES |
dc.format | application/pdf | |
dc.language | spa | |
dc.publisher | www.unbosque.edu.co | es-ES |
dc.relation | https://revistas.unbosque.edu.co/index.php/CHP/article/view/3050/2356 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Abu-Akel, A. (2018). The case for simulation theory and theory- theory as interaction accounts of theory of Mind. Constructivist Foundations, 14(1), 33-34. https://constructivist.info/14/1/033.abu-akel | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Apperly, I. A. (2008). Beyond simulation–theory and theory–theory: why social cognitive neuroscience should use its own concepts to study “Theory of Mind”. Cognition, 107(1), 266-283. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.019 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Ball, C. L., Smetana, J. G., & Sturge‐Apple, M. L. (2017). Following my head and my heart: Integrating preschoolers' empathy, theory of mind, and moral judgments. Child Development, 88(2), 597-611. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12605 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Barclay, P. (2015). Reputation. En Buss, D (Ed) The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, (pp. 810-828). Wiley. http://www.patbarclay.com/wp-content/uploads/2015/08/Barclay-Reputation-sending-version.pdf | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37-46. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Bloom, P., & German, T. P. (2000). Two reasons to abandon the false belief task as a test of theory of mind. Cognition, 77(1), 25-31. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(00)00096-2 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Butterfill, S. A., & Apperly, I. A. (2013). How to construct a minimal theory of mind. Mind & Language, 28(5), 606-637 https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12036 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Bzdok, D., Schilbach, L., Vogeley, K., Schneider, K., Laird, A. R., Langner, R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2012). Parsing the neural correlates of moral cognition: ALE meta-analysis on morality, theory of mind, and empathy. Brain Structure and Function, 217(4), 783-796. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0380-y | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Chakroff, A., Dungan, J., Koster-Hale, J., Brown, A., Saxe, R., & Young, L. (2016). When minds matter for moral judgment: intent information is neurally encoded for harmful but not impure acts. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(3), 476-484. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv131 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Cushman, F., Sheketoff, R., Wharton, S., & Carey, S. (20 13). The development of intent-based moral judgment. Cognition, 127(1), 6-21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.11.008 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/D'Esterre, A. P., Rizzo, M. T., & Killen, M. (2019). Unintentional and intentional falsehoods: The role of morally relevant theory of mind. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 177, 53-69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.07.013 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Fu, G., Xiao, W. S., Killen, M., & Lee, K. (2014). Moral judgment and its relation to second-order theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 50(8), 2085. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037077 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Geipel, J., Hadjichristidis, C., & Surian, L. (2016). Foreign language affects the contribution of intentions and outcomes to moral judgment. Cognition, 154, 34-39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.05.010 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Goenaga, J. (2016). Revisión sistemática sobre juicio moral en toma de decisiones morales (2005-2015). Tesis de Maestría en Psicología. Medellín: Departamento de Psicología, Universidad de Antioquia. | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Greene, J., & Haidt, J. (2002). How (and where) does moral judgment work? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(12), 517-523. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(02)02011-9 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293(5537), 2105-2108. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1062872 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814-834. https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511814273.055 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Killen, M., Mulvey, K. L., Richardson, C., Jampol, N., & Woodward, A. (2011). The accidental transgressor: Morally-relevant theory of mind. Cognition, 119(2), 197-215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.006 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Koster-Hale, J., Saxe, R., Dungan, J., & Young, L. L. (2013). Decoding moral judgments from neural representations of intentions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 201207992. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1207992110 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Kuntoro, I. A., Dwiputri, G., & Adams, P. (2018). The contribution of parenting style and theory of mind to the understanding of morally relevant theory of mind in Indonesian children. Diversity in Unity: Perspectives from Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, 83 - 89. https://doi.org/10.1201/9781315225302-11 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Lane, J. D., Wellman, H. M., Olson, S. L., LaBounty, J., & Kerr, D. C. (2010). Theory of mind and emotion understanding predict moral development in early childhood. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28(4), 871-889. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151009x483056 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Li, L., Rizzo, M. T., Burkholder, A. R., & Killen, M. (2017). Theory of mind and resource allocation in the context of hidden inequality. Cognitive Development, 43, 25-36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.02.001 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Margoni, F., & Surian, L. (2017). Children’s intention-based moral judgments of helping agents. Cognitive Development, 41, 46-64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.12.001 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Miller, S. A. (2009). Children’s understanding of second-order mental states. Psychological Bulletin, 135(5), 749. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016854 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Monin, B., Pizarro, D. A., & Beer, J. S. (2007). Deciding versus reacting: Conceptions of moral judgment and the reason-affect debate. Review of general psychology, 11(2), 99-111. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.11.2.99 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Moran, J. M. (2013). Lifespan development: The effects of typical aging on theory of mind. Behavioural Brain Research, 237, 32-40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.09.020 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Nobes, G., Panagiotaki, G., & Pawson, C. (2009). The influence of negligence, intention, and outcome on children’s moral judgments. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104(4), 382-397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.08.001 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Olson, K. R., & Spelke, E. S. (2008). Foundations of cooperation in young children. Cognition, 108(1), 222-231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.003 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Ortiz, D. S. U., Botero, M. G., & Tobón, O. E. A. (2010). Teoría de la mente: una revisión acerca del desarrollo del concepto. Revista Colombiana de Ciencias Sociales, 1(1), 28-37. https://www.funlam.edu.co/revistas/index.php/RCCS/article/view/1169 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Reddy, V., & Morris, P. (2004). Participants don’t need theories: knowing minds in engagement. Theory & Psychology, 14(5), 647-665. https://doi.org/10.1057/9780230234383_5 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Smetana, J. G., Jambon, M., Conry-Murray, C., & Sturge-Apple, M. L. (2012). Reciprocal Associations Between Young Children's Developing Moral Judgments and Theory of Mind. Developmental Psychology, 48(4), 1144-1155. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025891 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Sodian, B., Licata, M., Kristen‐Antonow, S., Paulus, M., Killen, M., & Woodward, A. (2016). Understanding of goals, beliefs, and desires predicts morally relevant theory of mind: A longitudinal investigation. Child Development, 87(4), 1221-1232. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12533 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Tirapu-Ustárroz, J., Pérez-Sayes, G., Erekatxo-Bilbao, M., & Pelegrín-Valero, C. (2007). ¿Qué es la teoría de la mente? Revista de Neurología, 44(8), 479-489. https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.4408.2006295 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Tomasello, M., & Vaish, A. (2013). Origins of human cooperation and morality. Annual review of psychology, 64, 231-255. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143812 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Vaish, A., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Young children selectively avoid helping people with harmful intentions. Child Development, 81(6), 1661-1669. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01500.x | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103-128. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Young, L., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., & Saxe, R. (2007). The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(20), 8235-8240. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0701408104 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Young, L., & Saxe, R. (2008). The neural basis of belief encoding and integration in moral judgment. Neuroimage, 40(4), 1912-1920. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.057 | |
dc.relation | /*ref*/Young, L., Camprodon, J. A., Hauser, M., Pascual-Leone, A., & Saxe, R. (2010). Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(15), 6753-6758. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0914826107 | |
dc.rights | Derechos de autor 2018 Cuadernos Hispanoamericanos de Psicología | es-ES |
dc.source | Cuadernos Hispanoamericanos de Psicología; Vol 18 No 2 (2018): Cuadernos Hispanoamercanos de Psicología; 1-16 | en-US |
dc.source | Cuadernos Hispanoamericanos de Psicología; ##issue.vol## 18 ##issue.no## 2 (2018): Cuadernos Hispanoamercanos de Psicología; 1-16 | es-AR |
dc.source | Cuadernos Hispanoamericanos de Psicología; Vol. 18 Núm. 2 (2018): Cuadernos Hispanoamercanos de Psicología; 1-16 | es-ES |
dc.source | 2346-0253 | |
dc.source | 1657-3412 | |
dc.subject | juicio moral | es-ES |
dc.subject | Teoría de la Mente (ToM) | es-ES |
dc.subject | Teoría de la Mente Moralmente Relevante (MoToM) | es-ES |
dc.subject | lectura intencional | es-ES |
dc.subject | tarea de falsa creencia | es-ES |
dc.title | Intentional Variation in Morally Relevant Theory of Mind (MoToM) Tasks | en-US |
dc.title | Variación intencional en tareas de Teoría de la Mente Moralmente relevante (MoToM) | es-ES |
dc.type | info:eu-repo/semantics/article | |
dc.type | info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion | |
dc.type | en-US | |
dc.type | es-ES |
Ficheros en el ítem
Ficheros | Tamaño | Formato | Ver |
---|---|---|---|
No hay ficheros asociados a este ítem. |